Mental Illness Research Paper
Public
Attitudes to Mental Illness Research Paper
1. DEFINITIONS
Mental illness is a broad
generic term which includes major mental illnesses such as schizophrenia,
bipolar affective disorders, common mental disorders such as anxiety and
depression, personality disorders, and various other conditions sometimes named
after etiology (for example, post-traumatic stress disorder), or after abnormal
behavior (for example, episodic dyscontrol syndrome). Therefore, when
discussing attitudes towards mental illness, it is important that individual
categories are accepted rather than broad generic terms.
1.1 ATTITUDES
Attitudes form a key
point of an individual’s thinking, especially toward things that matter to the
individual. Attitudes have several components—some a function of the dimensions
of personality traits, whereas others are a function of access. By assessing
attitudes, one can measure beliefs, but it is not always likely that these
beliefs can be turned into behavior. Thus, both cognitive and affective
components of these attitudes must be assessed, separately and together. These
attitudes do not always remain static and are influenced markedly by a number of
factors such as media, education, and societal conditions.
Sample Public Attitudes to Mental Illness Research Paper. Browse
other research paper examples and check the list
of research paper topics for
more inspiration. If you need a research paper written according to all the
academic standards, you can always turn to our experienced writers for help.
This is how your paper can get an A! Feel free to contact our research paper writing service for
professional assistance. We offer high-quality assignments for reasonable
rates.
Public
attitudes to mental illness and mentally ill individuals vary according to a
number of internal and external factors. In addition, these attitudes are
related to how the illness is perceived in itself, and its causation. The
portrayal of mentally ill individuals and psychiatrists in the written and
visual media are often negative. Such portrayals not only influence attitudes of
the general public but are also influenced in turn by the public’s attitudes.
Although some cultures may attribute special powers to the mentally ill, by and
large societies see mentally ill individuals as violent, frightening, and
dangerous, and mental illness as an incurable condition without distinguishing
between different types of mental illness. There is considerable research
evidence to suggest that common mental disorders such as depression and anxiety
may well affect up to one third of the adult population, but most of the
negative attitudes are linked with severe major mental illness such as
schizophrenia or bipolar affective illness. It can be argued that it is not the
illnesses per se which provoke negative attitudes, but what they represent
(i.e., ‘the outsiderness’) that is more frightening. These attitudes may or may
not always translate into behavior, but will significantly influence the
allocation of resources into the delivery of mental health services,
recruitment into various branches of mental health professions, and general
acceptance of mentally ill individuals by the larger community. In addition,
attitudes toward medication can prove to be negative.
1.2 STIGMA
Stigma is the expectation of
stereotypical and discrediting judgment of oneself by others in a particular
context. The term originates from Goffman’s (1963) definition of stigma, which
states that stigma originates when an individual, because of some attributes,
is disqualified from full social acceptance. Stigma, like the attributes, is
deeply embedded in the sociocultural milieu in which it originates and is thus
negotiable.
The Greeks in ancient times
used the word stigma to refer to bodily signs that exposed something unusual
and negative about the signifier. Further conceptualization of stigma has
included a mark that sets an individual apart (by virtue of mental illness in
this case), that links that individual to some undesirable characteristic
(perceived or real in cases of mental illness using violence, fear, and
aggression) and rejection, isolation, or discrimination against the individual
(e.g., at the workplace by virtue of the sick role and mental illness). Stigma
thus includes cognitive and behavioral components and can be applied to age,
race, sexual orientation, criminal behavior, and substance misuse, as well as
to mental illness. Stigmatizing reactions are always negative and related to
negative stereotypes. Stigma allows the society to extend the label of being
outside the society to all those who have negative stereotypes. Such an
exclusion may be seen as necessary for bifurcation of society, and will take
the same role as scapegoating. The definitions of sickness and sick roles will
change with the society, and stigma will also influence how distress is
expressed, what idioms are used, and how help is sought.
Stigma and negative attitudes
to mental illness are by and large universal. These are briefly discussed below
to highlight similarities prior to discussing various factors responsible for
these attitudes.
2. ATTITUDES IN EUROPE
The attitudes of public and
professionals alike across different countries of Europe have been shown to vary
enormously. In this section some of the key studies and the findings are
reported.
2.1 HISTORICAL
In ancient Greece, stigma and
shame were interlinked and these can be studied using theories of causation and
the balance within the culture between exclusion of mentally ill individuals
and the ritual means of cleansing and re-inclusion. The interwoven theories of
shame, stigma, pollution, and the erratic unpredictability of the behavior of
severely mentally ill individuals suggest that, in spite of scientific and
religious explanations, attitudes were generally negative. Simon (1992)
suggests that the ambivalence of the ancient Greeks can explain their attitudes
toward mental illness. The diseases of the mind were seen in parallel with
those of the body, and even the doctors were part and parcel of the same
cultural ambience of competition, shame, and fear of failure. During the
Medieval and Renaissance periods, mental illness was perceived as a result of
imbalance of various humors of the body, and related concepts of shame
contributed to this. As Christianity encouraged a God-fearing attitude to the
world, mental illness was seen as a sign that God was displeased and punishing
the individual, thereby making negative attitudes more likely and more
acceptable.
In the seventeenth and
eighteenth centuries attitudes shifted, but overall remained negative and in
turn provided entertainment to the visitors to Bedlam asylum where individual
visitors paid a penny to see the inmates.
2.2 CURRENT RECENT
In the nineteenth century, with
the establishment of various psychiatric asylums in the UK and the USA, there
was generally a growing perception among the lay public and physicians alike
that the mind is a function of the brain, thereby influencing a shift toward
more humane treatments—although these were still called moral therapy, thereby
giving it a moral religious tinge. The Athenian thinking on psyche has
influenced these attitudes. Porter (1987) proposed that the growing importance
of science and technology (among other things) was influential in channeling the
power of right thinking people in imposing the social norms. The men of the
church (also men of power) influenced public opinion, and informed attitudes and
behavior toward marginal social elements that would then become disturbed and
alien. Central state or market economy influenced the expectations which then
divided those who set and met the norms from those who did not.
In a survey, the British
Broadcasting Corporation (BBC 1957) reported that the public’s tolerance of
mentally ill individuals depended upon the circumstances in which contact was
made. More than three quarters of those surveyed were willing to mix with
mentally ill individuals in areas where low personal involvement occurred; only
half were willing to work with people with mental illness, whereas only a
quarter were agreeable to such an individual being in authority over them.
Attitudes toward mental illness and mentally ill individuals is influenced by a
number of external factors such as legal conditions, expectations of treatment,
etc.
In Turkey, for example, Eker
(1989) reported that paranoid schizophrenia was identified easily among various
groups. These attitudes influenced family perceptions, helpseeking, and caring
for the individual who is mentally ill, and are influenced by educational
status, gender, age, social class, and knowledge. Brandli (1999) observed that
in Switzerland stigma was more common in older nonurban males with low
education, and poorly informed. There were differences in public knowledge about
Alzheimer’s disease, depression, and other psychiatric conditions. The findings
also suggested that the general public was more accepting of seeking help from
their primary care physician rather than a psychiatrist. In Greece, a study
revealed that the younger, more educated, and higher social class individuals
saw mental illness as a psychosocial problem, whereas older people saw it as a
disorder of the nervous system (Lyketsos et al. 1985). Follow-up studies showed
an improvement in people’s attitudes. Religion has been shown to play a role in
forming these attitudes, but a sample from Israel (Shurka 1983) demonstrated
that attitudes were more likely to be mixed and inconsistent. From the UK, the
role of gender (Bhugra and Scott 1989) and ethnicity (Wolf et al. 1999) have
been shown to influence attitudes to mentally ill individuals. The attitudes of
other medical and nonmedical professions are also important, but are beyond the
scope of this research paper.
3. ATTITUDES IN AMERICA
Most of the studies from North
America have been carried out by social scientists and have illustrated a range
of attitudes to a number of conditions. Here only some of the important studies
will be discussed.
3.1 HISTORICAL
In America from the seventeenth
century onward, the traditional belief prevailed among Christians that madness
is often a punishment visited by God on the sinner, and this influenced
subsequent views and attitudes. Although both physical and mental illnesses
were seen as punishments, it was only in the latter that moral components were
seen, especially in the context of teachings from the Bible. A lack of faith
and persistence of sinful and lewd thoughts, were seen as key causation factors
in the development of insanity. In the early 1940s, Cumming and Cumming (1957),
in their classic study of lay public perceptions of the mentally ill,
demonstrated that, when asked to agree with the proposition that anyone in the
community could become mentally ill, as normal and abnormal occur on a
continuum, the whole educational package was rejected. An explanation was that
expression of shame and inferiority was seen as important.
In the 1940s, Ramsey and Seipp
(1948) demonstrated that subjects who had higher socioeconomic and educational
levels were less likely to view mental illness as a punishment for sins and or
poor living conditions. Yet they were less pessimistic about recovery. In the
1960s, studies from the USA were seen as either optimistic or pessimistic depending
upon whether the researcher saw changes in attitudes toward the mentally ill as
positive accepting, or negative rejecting. It is often difficult to ascertain
whether stigma or deviant behavior causes the rejection.
3.2 RECENT STUDIES
Star vignettes have been used
in determining public knowledge of mental illness, and public attitudes to
mental illness and mentally ill people (Rabkin 1974). Crocetti and Lemkau
(1963) used vignettes to demonstrate that virtually all the subjects in their
sample agreed with the question, ‘Do you think that people who are mentally ill
require a doctor’s care as people who have any other sort of illness do?’ In
this sample, low social class and low educational levels did not predict
negative attitudes. In another study using case vignettes, it was shown that
the largest increase in rejection rates occurred when a person had been
admitted to a mental hospital and this was seen as due not to the fact that
they were unable to help themselves, but that a psychiatrist or mental health
professional had confirmed their status as mentally ill (Phillips 1963). The
rejection appeared to be based on how visibly the behavior deviates from the
customary role expectations. Continuance of symptoms, and public visibility of
these symptoms, and behavior, influences attitudes and makes them more negative.
Social distance is the distance between mentally ill individuals and the lay
public on a number of parameters, especially in social interactions. It may
also show how individuals will employ mentally ill persons at different levels
of responsibility, thereby denoting acceptance or rejection. People may feel
reasonably comfortable with mentally ill individuals if they do not have to mix
with them on a regular basis. Meyer (1964) reported that three quarters of the
subjects in his sample were willing to work with mentally ill people, but only
44 percent would imagine falling in love with such a person. There is no doubt
that some illnesses are more stigmatizing than others, as is the severity of
the illness, presence of a diagnostic label, and availability of alternative
roles. In addition, attitudes can be influenced by the type of treatment and the
patient’s response to it. Dovidio et al. (1985) reported that people are
ambivalent in their attitude toward persons with psychological problems. In an
undergraduate class of 94 males and 81 females, the students were asked to give
their first impressions of mentally ill applicants in the process of college
applications. The results showed that individuals with mental illness were seen
favorably in terms of character and competence, but negatively in the context
of security and sociability.
Link et al. (1992) suggest that
patients’ perceptions of how others see them are extremely important. A mental
illness label gives personal relevance to an individual’s beliefs about how
most people respond to mental patients. The degree to which the individual with
mental illness expects to be rejected is associated with demoralization, income
loss, and unemployment in individuals labeled mentally ill, but not in those
without mental illness. Thus it appears that labeling activates beliefs that
lead to negative consequences. Patients may use such strategies to protect
themselves or their networks.
Such stigmatized attitudes are
also reflected in relationships with other outsider groups, such as the
homeless, and influence portrayals of negative spoken and visual media (see Fink
and Tasman 1992).
4. ATTITUDES ELSEWHERE IN THE WORLD
Attitudes to the mentally ill
from other societies and cultural settings are also influenced by a number of
factors. Studies on the topic vary tremendously in their methods and data
access. The beliefs about mentally ill people and toward mental illness are
likely to be related to types of illness prevalent in that group.
4.1 HISTORICAL
In ancient Ayurvedic texts, the
description of mental illness also suggested physical forms of treatment. In
addition, diet and seasons were reported as playing key roles in the genesis of
such conditions. This approach allowed the locus of control to be shifted to
some degree away from the individual, thereby making that individual less
responsible and less likely to be stigmatized.
4.2 CURRENT
Ilechukwu (1988), in a survey
of 50 male and 50 female psychiatric outpatients in Lagos, was able to
demonstrate that some patients did believe in supernatural causes of neuroses
and disorders, but psychosocial causes were cited most commonly. The attitudes
and beliefs of patients are important, but beyond the scope of this research
paper.
Wig et al. (1980), in a
three-site study from India, Sudan, and the Philippines, found that, when asked
to comment on case vignettes, community leaders were able to identify mental
retardation (in all sites), alcohol and drug-related problems (in the Sudanese
and Philippines areas), and acute psychosis (in India). Thus it appears that
there are cultural differences in the identification of different clinical
conditions. They also reported that attitudes toward mentally ill people were
more negative in India compared with the other two sites. Thus the studies can
be used to establish the needs of the general public and will allow planners to
develop appropriate services.
Verghese and Beig (1974), in a
survey from South India, reported that over 40 percent of Muslim and Christian
respondents reported that marriage can help mental illness, although only 20
percent of Hindus shared this belief. Virtually no respondents reported
believing in evil spirits. The most commonly recognized causes mentioned were
emotional factors, including excessive thinking. In respondents over the age of
40, one fifth saw mental illness as God’s punishment. Christians were again more
likely than Hindus to fear mentally ill people (although this fear disappeared
with education), and yet the Christians believed strongly in the possibility of
a complete cure. Nearly three-quarters of Hindus believed that the moon
influences mental illness. These are interesting findings in that they address
religious differences in public attitudes.
5. REASONS FOR NEGATI E ATTITUDES
Reactions to any taboo or outsider
group depend upon a number of factors. These include the frequency of the
actual or anticipated behavioral events, intensity and visibility of such
behavior, and circumstances and location of such behavior on the one hand, and
personal factors on the other. In this section we focus on the latter.
5.1 AGE
Several studies have shown that
older people tend to have more negative attitudes toward mentally ill people.
The reasons for this are many. Older people, in spite of their life
experiences, are generally more conservative and equally rejecting of behavior
which is seen as odd and alien. The role of age is likely to be mediated by
other factors such as education, social and economic class, etc.
5.2 TYPES OF ILLNESS
Attitudes towards people with
schizophrenia are likely to differ from those reported toward people with
depression or personality disorder. This may reflect the stereotypic images of
the condition or fear related to the condition. It may also be due to previous
knowledge about the illness. The perceived causative etiological factors also
play a role in attitudes.
5.3 GENDER
Males tend to have more
negative attitudes and, as noted above, are also more likely to be rejected
when they suffer mental illness. Females may be more sympathetic, for a number
of reasons. Other studies of attitudes toward other alien groups also
demonstrate that females are more positive. They are also more likely to be
carers, and may be the first to contact psychiatric services on behalf of
individuals. For women, change in role after mental illness is likely to
produce more stigma. Yet they are more likely to be admitted with more
‘masculine’ illnesses such as personality disorder and drug abuse. The gender
roles in the context of illness may well play a role in generating negative
attitudes.
5.4 RELIGIOUS BELIEFS
Some studies have demonstrated
that Christians tend to have more negative attitudes toward mental illness, but
again this is not a consistent finding. The individual subject’s level of
religiosity and depth of religious values must be studied rather than simple
religious ascription.
5.5 EDUCATIONAL STATUS
The effects of education on
attitudes are mixed (Bhugra 1989). Some studies have related negative attitudes
clearly with low educational status, whereas others have failed to show such
association, or showed that highly educated subjects held more negative
attitudes, including those studying professionally.
5.6 PROFESSIONS
Medical students may hold more
negative and stereotypical attitudes, and other branches of medicine too have
been shown to have negative attitudes to mentally ill people (e.g.,
schizophrenics), those with odd behavior (e.g., deliberate self-harm), and
psychiatry as a profession. The psychiatrist is often lampooned and seen as a
‘head shrinker’ or a modern-day witchdoctor. There are individuals who are well
educated and belong to a high socioeconomic class and yet hold negative
attitudes.
5.7 OTHERS
Some ethnic groups such as
African-Caribbeans in the UK, and Hispanics, Asian-Americans, and
Mexican-Americans in the USA have been found to have more negative attitudes
toward mentally ill individuals or be far more restrictive in their description
of etiology of mental illness compared to the white majority population. Nonacculturated
individuals appeared to have more old-fashioned attitudes (i.e., negative
stereotypes).
These are some of the complex
set of factors that influence attitudes and stereotypes.
6. EDUCATIONAL INTERVENTIONS
Educational interventions in
order to reduce stigma toward mentally ill individuals and foster positive
attitudes towards mental illness and those who are mentally ill are based on
several levels of education. One-off educational programs and fact sheets on any
illness are not likely to produce long-term changes. There is considerable
evidence in the literature to suggest that education, if given at an
appropriate and stable level, and repeated as required, will produce changes
that can be sustained. In addition, educational interventions must target
specific populations, bearing in mind their age, gender, ethnic composition,
primary language, educational attainments, social class, etc.
Wolff et al. (1999) reported on
the results of followup of neighbors, where two group homes for people with
mental illness had been set up and one set of neighbors had received extensive
education. They found that although the public education intervention may have
had at best only a modest effect on knowledge, behavior toward the mentally ill
residents changed. There was a decrease in fear and exclusion, and increased
levels of social contact in the experimental area. They observed that
educational intervention per se did not in itself lead directly to a reduction
in fearful attitudes, whereas contact with patients did. Thus, any campaign
which encourages subjects to increase contact with mentally ill individuals may
prove to be more successful. The educational programs must be paced slowly and
in a sustainable manner. The patients and their carers must be involved in
planning these interventions without turning the whole exercise into a circus.
Any educational intervention
must target the intervention either at the group most at risk of negative
attitudes or at those already having negative attitudes. Repeated packages and
adequate time for consultation and discussion will influence attitudes. Using
small groups with experiential teaching is more likely to be successful
compared with seminars or lectures with large groups. Any educational campaign
must be sustained and momentum maintained. The effect on attitudes following
several interventions is more sustained, and greater than the sum of the
individual effects. Using a number of strategies, involving participation on the
part of the public, carers, and patients, can influence attitudes.
The educational packages and
interventions aimed at improving attitudes will, in the long run, influence
resource allocation into mental health, improve recruitment, and move toward
acceptance of community care. Such interventions can reduce fear, make
expectations more realistic, and prevent attitudes from hardening. However,
these interventions must be clear, focused, and appropriate, based upon the
needs of the group that is being educated rather than on perceptions of need by
the professionals. These packages must deal with alienation experienced by
patients at different levels and in different settings, such as housing,
employment, and social settings.
7. CONCLUSIONS
The attitudes of the public
toward mentally ill individuals reflect prevalent social norms and mores.
Expectations from psychiatric services, response to treatment, type of
treatment, type of mental illness, and inaccessibility to treatment are
external factors that will influence public attitudes. Age, gender, personality
traits, and social and economic class are some of the personal factors that
will influence these attitudes. These attitudes will not always translate into
behavior, but one will be influenced by the other. Labeling of mental illness
and the perception of mentally ill individuals as dangerous along with
associated fear will all influence attitude formation. Any educational
intervention must include some or all of these factors. Negative attitudes will
influence helpseeking as well as compliance. These attitudes may well be linked
to illness, but also to stereotypes of the illness, and to associated or
perceived impairment as a result of that illness. These negative attitudes will
also influence rejection of any preventative strategies the psychiatric
profession may wish to advocate.
Bibliography:
1. BBC
1957 The Hurt Mind: An Audience Research Report. BBC, London
2. Bhugra
D 1989 Attitudes towards mental illness. Acta Psychiatrica Scandina ica 80:
1–12
3. Bhugra
D, Scott J 1989 Public image of psychiatry: A pilot study. Psychiatric Bulletin
of the Royal College of Psychiatry 13 : 330–3
4. Brandli
H 1999 The image of mental illness in Switzerland. In: Guimon M J, Fischer W,
Sartorius N (eds.) The Image of Madness. Karger, Basle, Switzerland
5. Crocetti
G M, Lemkau P V 1963 Public opinion of psychiatric home care in an urban area.
American Journal of Public Health 53: 409–17
6. Cumming
E, Cumming J 1957 Closed Ranks: An Experiment in Mental Health Education.
Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA
7. Dovidio
J, Fishbane R, Sibicky M 1985 Perceptions of people with psychological
problems. Psychological Reports 57: 1263–70
8. Eker
D 1985 Attitudes of Turkish and American clinicians and Turkish psychology
students toward mental patients. International Journal of Psychiatry 31:
223–229
9. Fink
P, Tasman A 1992 Stigma and Mental Illness. APA Press, Washington, DC
10. Goffman
E 1963 Stigma. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ
11. Guimon
J, Fischer W, Sartorius N 1999 The Image of Madness. Karger, Basle, Switzerland
12. Ilechukwu
S 1988 Interrelationship of beliefs about mental illness psychiatric diagnosis
and mental health care delivery among Africans. International Journal of Social
Psychiatry 34: 200–6
13. Link
B, Cullen F, Mirotznik J, Struening E 1992 The consequences of stigma for
persons with mental illness. In: Fink P J, Tasman A (eds.) Stigma and Mental
Illness 1st edn. APA Press, Washington, DC
14. Lyketsos
G, Mouyas A, Malliori M, Lyketsos C 1985 Opinion of public and patients about
mental illness and psychiatric care in Greece. British Journal of Clinical and
Social Psychiatry 3: 59–66
15. Meyer
J K 1964 Attitudes towards mental illness in a Maryland community. Public
Health Reports 79: 769–72
16. Phillips
D 1964 Rejection of the mentally ill: The influence of behavior and sex.
American Sociological Re iew 29: 679–89
17. Porter
R 1987 Mind Forg’d Manacles. Athlone, London
18. Rabkin
J 1974 Public attitudes towards mental illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin 10: 9–23
19. Ramsey
G, Seipp M 1948 Attitudes and opinions concerning mental illness. Psychiatric
Quarterly 22: 428–44
20. Shurka
E 1983 Attitudes of Israeli Arabs towards the mentally ill. International
Journal of Social Psychiatry 29: 95–100
21. Simon
B 1992 Shame, stigma and mental illness in Ancient Greece. In: Fink P J, Tasman
A (eds.) Stigma and Mental Illness, 1st edn. APA Press, Washington, DC
22. Verghese
A, Beig A 1974 Public attitudes towards mental illness: The Vellore study.
Indian Journal of Psychiatry 16: 8–18
23. Wig
N N, Suleiman M A, Routledge R et al. 1980 Community reactions to mental
disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 61: 111–26
24. Wolff
G, Pathare S, Craig T, Left J 1999 Public education for community care. In:
Guimon J, Fischer W, Sartorious N (eds.) The Image of Madness. Karger, Basle,
Switzerland
Social
Determinants of Mental Health Research Paper
View sample mental health research paper on
social determinants of mental health. Browse other research paper examples for more
inspiration. If you need a thorough research paper written according to all the
academic standards, you can always turn to our experienced writers for help.
This is how your paper can get an A! Feel free to contact our writing service
for professional assistance. We offer high-quality assignments for reasonable
rates.
HISTORICAL BACKGROUND
The origin
of modern research on the social determinants of mental health and disorders is
often traced to the work of Emile Durkheim, who demonstrated that cross-national
variations in suicide rates reflected differences in social conditions rather
than the characteristics of individuals. This line of reasoning – that social
structures exert profound influence on the lives and well-being of individuals
– has dominated thinking about the social determinants of mental disorders ever
since. For example, the work of Robert Faris and H. Warren Dunham demonstrated
that the prevalence of psychosis was higher in the poor and slum neighborhoods
of Chicago than in wealthier districts of the city. Similarly, the research of
Alexander Leighton and colleagues found that rates of mental disorder in
Nigeria and Nova Scotia, Canada were highest in communities experiencing social
disorganization. Durkheim’s influence is apparent in more recent research on
the characteristics of neighborhoods and variations in physical and mental
health, a growing interest in the concept of social capital (see below), as
well as, more generally, in research on the association of socioeconomic status
and well-being.
STRESS MODELS OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY
The
‘stress-adversity’ model of psychopathology, as formulated by Bruce Dohrenwend
(2000), proposes that the degree to which environments present danger and
hardship to individuals will be positively associated with risk for
psychopathology. The association between environment and psychopathology will
be reduced by the degree to which individuals have the ability to respond to
and cope with the adversities.
How social
groups come to be at increased risk of stress is central to considerations of
the social determinants of mental health and disorders. As suggested by Leonard
Pearlin (1989), relative well-being is associated with ‘the structured
arrangements of people’s lives and by the repeated experiences that stem from
these arrangements.’ Thus, the social positions of particular populations put
them at differential risk of stress. For example, the stress associated with
becoming unemployed is different for a member of a poor family than it is for a
member of a wealthy family. Further, being poor often means living in a
crowded, polluted, and dangerous neighborhood, which is far more stressful,
physically and psychologically, than being wealthy and able to afford living in
a quiet suburb with tree-lined streets. Finally, differential access to
effective medical care will have consequences for the relative well-being of
social groups.
The ‘stress-diathesis’ model
builds on the stress adversity model by positing that risk for psychopathology
is produced by an interaction between environmental stressors and individual
vulnerability. One must not suppose, however, that individual vulnerability
negates the notion of social determinants of well-being (Monroe and Simons,
1991). It is likely that individual vulnerability to common physical and mental
disorders is evenly distributed in large populations. Therefore, exposure of
subpopulations to different levels of stress will result in social
differentials in the expression of those vulnerabilities. In view of this, one could
say that the term ‘social determinants’ is an overly simplistic consideration
of causality. It would be more accurate to refer to ‘environment–gene
interactions’ as a primary source of social differentials in health and
well-being. Even this is something of an oversimplification. Environments
contain features that may mitigate or intensify the effects of social
adversities. Thus, residents of a neighborhood with a relatively high degree of
social capital (see below) may be less affected by an economic crisis; in
contrast, residents of a neighborhood wracked by violence may be less capable
of resilience in the face of a natural disaster.
SOCIAL RISK FACTORS FOR MENTAL DISORDERS
GENDER
Gender, which may be thought of
as the social roles designated for men and women in different sociocultural
settings, carries with it differential risk for a range of mental disorders.
For example, women are two to three times more likely than men to experience
depression, and postnatal depression has been recognized as a significant
problem worldwide. In most societies, completed suicide rates among men are
much higher than among women, but rates of attempted suicide are much higher in
women. Men are many times more likely to abuse substances, particularly
alcohol. While it is likely that gender differences for common mental disorders
are at least partially due to sociocultural factors, biological factors likely
also play an important role.
SOCIOECONOMIC
STATUS
Socioeconomic status (SES),
which is variously measured by levels of income, educational attainment,
occupation, and neighborhood characteristics (see below), exerts a profound
influence on health status. On average, people of higher SES have rates of
mortality and morbidity that are significantly lower than people of lower SES.
The same relationship is true for mental disorders. For example, Ronald Kessler
and colleagues (2003) have found that being unemployed, having less than 12
years of education, and having a low income are all associated with elevated
prevalence of depression in a representative sample of adults in the United
States. Findings from the Whitehall study (Stansfeld et al., 2003), which
examined the health of civil servants in the United Kingdom, also supports the
notion of social inequalities in depression: Higher-grade civil servants had
lower levels of depression than those in the lower grades. Other research
demonstrates that the same relationship is true for psychosis: Low SES is
associated with elevated rates of the disorder.
For a long time, there has been
a debate over whether this pattern is the result of social drift or social
causation. According to social drift theory, elevated rates of mental disorder
are found among low SES groups because mental disorders impair the ability of
individuals to raise themselves out of that status or limit the ability of
individuals to maintain their higher status. Thus, mentally ill individuals
drift into low SES. In contrast, the social causation theory suggests that risk
for mental disorder is heightened for low SES individuals because of the
stressful social environments in which they live.
Probably the best research to
test the validity of these two competing theories was conducted by Bruce
Dohrenwend and colleagues (1992). In an investigation of nearly 5000
Israeli-born adults, they found that (1) persons who had not graduated high
school had rates of depression that were higher than persons who had graduated
either high school or college; and, (2) educational status had no association
with rates for schizophrenia. To further examine the relation between social
status and mental disorder, Dohrenwend and colleagues also looked at rates of
depression among adults of European (advantaged) and North African
(disadvantaged) backgrounds. The results mirrored those for educational status:
Those from disadvantaged backgrounds had elevated rates of depression, but
rates of schizophrenia were the same for advantaged and disadvantaged groups.
Thus, this research suggests that the social causation theory accounts for
subpopulation inequalities in rates of depression, while the social drift
theory accounts for subpopulation inequalities in rates of schizophrenia.
One must not assume that the
various measures of SES, income and education in particular, are
interchangeable. As demonstrated by Araya and colleagues (2003), the predictive
power of these variables is very much context-dependent. They found an inverse
relation between levels of education and the prevalence of common mental
disorders in Chile, while in the United Kingdom level of income, but not
education, was associated with prevalence, and in the United States both income
and education were found to have significant associations with prevalence of
common mental disorders.
RACE/ETHNICITY
Too often, social status as
measured by membership in racial/ethnic groups is seen as a proxy for
socioeconomic status. However, the relationship is much more complex. For
example, although African-Americans in the United States are a socially
disadvantaged group, their rates of depression and suicide are lower than the
majority white population (in contrast to predictions based on SES). Other
evidence suggests that African-Americans have higher rates of depressive symptoms
and that their risk for persistent mood and anxiety disorders is higher (in
keeping with predictions based on SES). The AESOP study (Fearon et al., 2006)
reports that incidence rates of psychosis among the African-Caribbean and black
African populations in the United Kingdom are substantially higher than in
white Britons, a finding that suggests that membership in a racial or ethnic
minority may confer risk for mental disorder, independent of SES. In general,
research from Australia, the United Kingdom, the Netherlands, Denmark, and
Sweden support these findings in that immigrants, especially those from racial
or ethnic backgrounds that are different from the host countries, are at
increased risk for psychosis.
PSYCHOSOCIAL
ENVIRONMENTS
SOCIAL
CAPITAL
The concept of social capital
emerges from the work of Durkheim in that it looks to features of social
environments to explain the collective behavior of individuals. Specifically,
social capital may be defined as those properties of social units (e.g.,
neighborhoods, communities, cities, or provinces) that include, as defined by
De Silva and colleagues (2005), ‘the quantity and quality of formal and
informal social interactions, civic participation, norms of reciprocity, and
trust in others.’ Research literature has demonstrated a strong and positive
association between levels of social capital and the health status of
communities, and there is growing evidence of an inverse association between
social capital and risk for common mental disorders such as depression and
anxiety. However, difficulties in precise definition and measurement of social
capital must be overcome before it is possible to develop public mental health
policies based on the concept of social capital.
NEIGHBORHOODS
There is now a large body of
evidence demonstrating the association of neighborhood characteristics (e.g.,
proportion of households living in poverty) with physical health. There is also
evidence that the collective level of depressive symptoms is influenced by the
characteristics of neighborhoods. Indeed, a 2006 study by Cohen and colleagues
shows that, compared to older residents of middle and high-income
neighborhoods, older residents of low-income neighborhoods are less likely to
respond to even the best of antidepressant treatment. As noted above, Faris and
Dunham found high rates of psychosis in the inner city of Chicago.
Additionally, a recent meta-analysis by John McGrath and colleagues at the
University of Queensland (2004) suggests that relatively high rates of
schizophrenia are associated with urban residence.
OCCUPATION
AND SOCIAL STATUS
There is increasing evidence
that social inequalities in well-being are the consequence of psychological
processes. For example, the Whitehall study (Stansfeld et al., 2003) suggests
that psychosocial work environments (e.g., the extent to which one may make
decisions and use skills creatively) were more important than socioeconomic
status in determining risk for depression. More generally, research by Michael
Marmot (2004) suggests that subjective social status, that is, the perception
of one’s relative position in the social order, accounts for much of the social
gradient in health and well-being.
RAPID
SOCIAL CHANGE AND SOCIAL DISORGANIZATION
Durkheim associated rapid
social change (e.g., political and economic upheavals) with what he termed as
‘anomic’ suicide – suicide caused by a collective experience of chaos and/or
loss of meaning and purpose. The validity of the concept can be found in a
number of examples. As a result of decades of political violence, suicide rates
in Sri Lanka have gone from being among the lowest to among the highest in the
world, particularly among young adults (Somasundaram, 2007). The startling
increase of suicides in Japan since 1998 has been attributed to a range of
economic factors, including unemployment, bankruptcy, and debt (Curtin, 2004).
Gender inequities, as well as economic and social changes, are often cited to
explain the high rates of suicide among young women in rural China. Perhaps the
most dramatic example of anomic suicide is found among the indigenous peoples
of the world, who have experienced massive social and cultural dislocations for
hundreds of years. In Micronesia and Australia, for example, high rates of
suicide and self-harm among young men are likely the result of social changes
that have eroded traditional cultural activities and social structures that
helped to guide this age group through the difficult transition to adulthood.
Difficult and rapid social
transformations are often associated with increased rates of substance abuse,
alcohol-related problems, and suicide. Evidence of this is found, again, among
the indigenous peoples of the world; high rates of alcoholism are found among
indigenous groups in such disparate places as Australia, Taiwan, and North
America.
Rates of mortality in Russia
have gone through dramatic changes since the dissolution of the Soviet Union: a
sharp increase immediately after 1991, substantial improvement between 1994 and
1998, and another decline after 1998. The result is that life expectancy in
Russia (66 years) is alarmingly shorter than in the developed nations of the
world ( 78 years). To a large degree, the overall decline in the health status
of the Russian population is due to alcohol abuse and related deaths, as well
as violence. Since 1991, the rate of suicide in Russia has remained one of the
highest in the world; it is also presumed that high levels of depression have
contributed to high levels of alcohol abuse and suicide. Again, the indigenous
peoples of the world provide a shocking example: Throughout the world, their
life expectancies are much shorter – almost 20 years shorter in Australia, for
example – than the general populations in which they live.
Globalization, specifically the
spread of Western media and cultural values, has been associated with the
appearance of anorexia nervosa in Hong Kong and other cities in China. Research
in the late 1990s demonstrated an association between eating disorders among
female Chinese high school students and their relative exposure to Western
media and values. In Hong Kong, a highly Westernized city, the prevalence of
eating disorders was high, while in the city of Shenzen and in rural Hunan the
prevalence was moderate and low, respectively. Research from Fiji provides even
stronger evidence of the causal relationship between the images portrayed in
Western media and eating disorders. Just prior to the introduction of
television (with programming primarily from the United States), a survey showed
that female Fijian high school students had very low levels of eating
disorders. Three years after the introduction of television, the same survey
was administered among a comparable group of students. This time the
respondents reported much higher levels of disordered eating behaviors. The
change was attributed to the introduction of television and the pervasive
images of women who were exceedingly thin (Becker, 2004).
VIOLENCE
AND TRAUMA
There is now a large body of
evidence that links the experiences of violence and trauma to risk for
depression and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), in particular. The
sociopolitical context of the refugee experience, predisplacement and post
displacement, is associated with refugee mental health. Conflict, war, and
disaster situations impact on fundamental family and community dynamics,
resulting in profound negative changes at a collective level. Vietnamese and
Cambodian victims of political violence and torture have been found to suffer
from elevated rates of these disorders. Under the rule of the Taliban in
Afghanistan, women suffered from high rates of depression and anxiety as a
result of the extreme social restrictions under which they were forced to live.
Indeed, there is extensive evidence from throughout the world about the mental
health consequences of violence against women. The trauma of natural disasters
– such as the tsunami that struck Aceh, Indonesia in 2004, earthquakes in China
and India, or hurricanes in the southern United States – has been linked to
increased rates of depression and PTSD. In sum, experiencing violence and/or
trauma substantially increases the risk for mental distress.
CONCLUSION
There is strong evidence that
links the social conditions in which people live and their psychological
well-being. Socioeconomic status, characteristics of neighborhoods, exposure to
violence, membership in racial or ethnic minorities, gender, and rapid social
change all influence psychological well-being and confer differential risk for
a range of mental disorders.
Bibliography:
1. Becker AE (2004) Television,
disordered eating, and young women in Fiji: Negotiating body image and identity
during rapid social change. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry 28(4): 533–559.
2. Cohen A (1999) The Mental
Health of Indigenous People: An International Overview. Geneva, Switzerland:
World Health Organization.
3. Cohen A, Houck PR, Szanto K, et
al. (2006) Social inequalities in response to antidepressant treatment in older
adults. Archives of General Psychiatry 63(1): 50–56.
4. Curtin JS (2004) Suicide also
rises in land of rising sun. Asia Times Online July 28.
http://www.atimes.com/atimes/Japan/FG28Dh01. html (accessed October 2007).
5. De Silva MJ, McKenzie K,
Harpham T, and Huttly SR (2005) Social capital and mental illness: A systematic
review. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 59(8): 619–627.
6. Dohrenwend BP, Levav I, Shrout
PE, et al. (1992) Socioeconomic status and psychiatric disorders: The
causation-selection issue. Science 255: 946–952.
7. Durkheim E (1996) Suicide. New
York: Free Press.
8. Fearon P, Kirkbride JB, Morgan
C, et al. (2006) Incidence of schizophrenia and other psychoses in ethnic
minority groups: Results from the MRC AESOP Study. Psychological Medicine
36(11): 1541–1550.
9. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler
O, et al. (2003) The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: Results from
the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Journal of the American
Medical Association 289(23): 3095–3105.
10. Lee S and Lee AM (2000)
Disordered eating in three communities of China: a comparative study of female
high school students in Hong Kong, Shenzhen, and rural Hunan. International
Journal of Eating Disorders 27: 317–332.
11. Marmot M (2004) The Status
Syndrome: How Social Standing Affects Our Health and Longevity. New York: Times
Books.
12. McGrath J, Saha S, Welham J, et
al. (2004) A systematic review of the incidence of schizophrenia: The
distribution of rates and the influence of sex, urbanicity, migrant status, and
methodology. BMC Medicine 2: 13.
13. Pearlin LI (1989) The
sociological study of stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 30:
241–256.
14. Phillips MR, Li X, and Zhang Y
(2002) Suicide rates in China, 1995–1999. Lancet 359: 835–840.
15. Pridemore WA and Spivak AL
(2003) Patterns of suicide mortality in Russia. Suicide and Life Threatening
Behavior 33(2): 132–150.
16. Shkolnikov V, McKee M, and Leon
DA (2001) Changes in life expectancy in Russia in the mid-1990s. Lancet
357(9260): 917–921.
17. Somasundaram D (2007) Collective
trauma in Northern Sri Lanka: A qualitative psychosocial-ecological study.
International Journal of Mental Health Systems 1:5.
18. Stansfeld SA, Head J, Fuhrer R,
Wardle J, and Cattell V (2003) Social inequalities in depressive symptoms and
physical functioning in the Whitehall II study: Exploring a common cause
explanation. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 57(5): 361–367.
19. Araya R, Lewis G, Rojas G, and
Fritsch R (2003) Education and income: Which is more important for mental
health? Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 57(7): 501–505.
20. Berkman LF, Glass T, Brissette
I, and Seeman TE (2000) From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new
millennium. Social Science and Medicine 51(6): 843–857.
21. Cantor-Graae E (2005)
Schizophrenia and migration: A meta-analysis and review. American Journal of
Psychiatry 162(1): 12–24.
22. Faris RE and and Dunham HW
(1939) Mental Disorders in Urban Areas: an Ecological study of schizophrenia
and other psychoses. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago press.
23. Tsuang MT, Bar JL, Stone WS,
and Faraone SV (2004) Gene-environment interactions in mental disorders. World
Psychiatry 3(2): 73–83.
Comments
Post a Comment